New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Ignoring cues for alcohol and fast food is hard -- but is it out of our control?

Date:
July 8, 2019
Source:
University of New South Wales
Summary:
A psychology experiment has shown why it can be so hard to direct our attention away from cues that might lead to behavior we'd like to avoid, like drinking alcohol and eating unhealthy food.
Share:
FULL STORY

Have you ever tried to stay away from fast food, but found hard-to-ignore signals that represent its availability -- like neon lights and ads -- are everywhere?

If you're stressed, tired or otherwise straining your brain power, you may find it harder to ignore cues in the environment that signal something rewarding.

That's what a UNSW Sydney experiment by a group of psychologists -- published today in Psychological Science -- has shown.

"We knew already that participants find it hard to ignore cues that signal a large reward," says study lead Dr Poppy Watson at UNSW.

But this experiment showed -- for the first time -- that ignoring these cues became harder as soon as participants had to perform a task while also holding other information in their memory.

"We have a set of control resources that are guiding us and helping us suppress these unwanted signals of reward. But when those resources are taxed, these become more and more difficult to ignore."

Up until now, researchers didn't know whether people's general inability to ignore reward cues is just something we have no control over or whether we do use our executive control processes to constantly work against distractions. But now it's become clear that the latter is the case -- although unfortunately this resource is limited.

Executive control is a term for all cognitive processes that allow us to pay attention, organise our life, focus, and regulate our emotions.

"Now that we have evidence that executive control processes are playing an important role in suppressing attention towards unwanted signals of reward, we can begin to look at the possibility of strengthening executive control as a possible treatment avenue for situations like addiction," says Dr Watson.

In the experiment, participants looked at a screen that contained various shapes including a colourful circle. They were told they could earn money if they successfully located and looked at the diamond shape, but that if they looked at the coloured circle -- the distractor -- they would not receive the money.

They were also told that the presence of a blue circle meant they'd gain a higher amount of money (if they completed the diamond task) than the presence of an orange circle. The scientists then used eye tracking to measure where on the screen participants were looking.

"To manipulate the ability of participants to control their attention resources, we asked them to do this task under conditions of both high memory load and low memory load," Dr Watson says.

In the high-memory load version of the experiment, participants were asked to memorise a sequence of numbers in addition to locating the diamond, meaning they had fewer attention resources available to focus on the diamond task.

"Study participants found it really difficult to stop themselves from looking at cues that represented the level of reward -- the coloured circles -- even though they were paid to try and ignore them," Dr Watson says.

"Crucially, the circles became harder to ignore when people were asked to also memorise numbers: under high memory load, participants looked at the coloured circle associated with the high reward around 50% of the time, even though this was entirely counterproductive."

Limited resources

The findings demonstrate that people need full access to cognitive control processes to try and suppress unwanted signals of reward in the environment.

"This is especially relevant for circumstances where people are trying to ignore cues and improve their behaviour, e.g. consuming less alcohol or fast food," says Dr Watson.

"There's this strong known link between where your attention is and what you eventually do, so if you find it hard to focus your attention away from reward cues, it's even harder to act accordingly.

Stress

That also explains why people might find it harder to focus on dieting or beating an addiction if they are under a lot of stress.

"Constant worrying or stress is the equivalent to the high-memory load scenario of our experiment, impacting on people's ability to use their executive control resources in a way that's helping them manage unwanted cues in the environment."

Dr Watson advises people to try and be strategic about exposure to cues.

"If you are under a lot of cognitive pressure (stress, or tiredness) you should really try and avoid situations where you'll be tempted by signals. You need to be in the right frame of mind to be in a situation where you can stop yourself from getting distracted and going down a path where you don't want to go," she says.

Treatment

The researchers now want to look at how executive control can be strengthened -- and if that presents an opportunity for situations like drug rehab.

"Our research suggests that if you strengthen executive control you should have better outcomes. Some studies have already demonstrated that training executive control can reduce the likelihood that you will eat chocolate or drink alcohol.

"And in the clinic, training attentional focus away from pictures of alcohol towards soft drinks has been shown to reduce relapse in alcoholic patients," Dr Watson says.

"However, the exact mechanisms of how this works are still unclear and we need more research to figure out how exactly we can use executive control to our advantage."


Story Source:

Materials provided by University of New South Wales. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Poppy Watson, Daniel Pearson, Michelle Chow, Jan Theeuwes, Reinout W. Wiers, Steven B. Most, Mike E. Le Pelley. Capture and Control: Working Memory Modulates Attentional Capture by Reward-Related Stimuli. Psychological Science, 2019; 095679761985596 DOI: 10.1177/0956797619855964

Cite This Page:

University of New South Wales. "Ignoring cues for alcohol and fast food is hard -- but is it out of our control?." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 8 July 2019. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190708092853.htm>.
University of New South Wales. (2019, July 8). Ignoring cues for alcohol and fast food is hard -- but is it out of our control?. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 21, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190708092853.htm
University of New South Wales. "Ignoring cues for alcohol and fast food is hard -- but is it out of our control?." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190708092853.htm (accessed December 21, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES