New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Positive emotions plus deep sleep equals longer-lasting perceptual memories

Date:
February 3, 2025
Source:
RIKEN
Summary:
Researchers have uncovered how perceptual memories linked to positive emotions, such as joy or happiness, are strengthened during sleep. The study could help scientists understand the neurological basis for overcoming conditions like drug addiction.
Share:
FULL STORY

Researchers at the RIKEN Center for Brain Science (CBS) have uncovered how perceptual memories linked to positive emotions, such as joy or happiness, are strengthened during sleep. The study, published in the scientific journal Neuron, could help scientists understand the neurological basis for overcoming conditions like drug or sexual addiction.

Why do emotional events, whether positive or negative, create strong, long-lasting memories of external information such as music, scene smells and textures received at the events? We know that sleep is essential for memory consolidation, the process that turns new events into memories, but do not know how it plays its role in memory enhancement. Experiments have shown that emotional events might be processed differently depending on the stage of sleep, but it is still unclear which part of sleep is more important for solidifying them, REM sleep -- the time during sleep when we dream vividly -- or non-REM sleep. The new study led by Masanori Murayama at RIKEN CBS set out to answer this question.

The researchers first established mouse equivalents of neutral and emotional events so that they could examine what the brain does differently during sleep when emotions are involved. In a neutral situation, male mice were allowed to explore only a smooth texture on day 1 of the experiment as a learning period and both grooved and smooth textures the next day as a testing period. When their memory of the smooth texture was kept until day 2, they preferentially explored the grooved texture, but not smooth, texture because mice innately like novel environments.

However, when the smooth texture was paired with a positive emotional experience -- interaction with a female mouse -- the memory of the texture lasted much longer. Even with a four-day interval, at day 5, the mice still preferred the smooth texture, showing that this experimental setup could produce emotionally enhanced texture memory in mice. As Murayama explains, "this is the first study to demonstrate perceptual memory enhancement through emotions in experimental animals. We were able to do so, and as a consequence, identified critical neural circuitry involved in perceptual memory enhancement."

The researchers identified the amygdala, the brain's emotional center, as key to this process. The amygdala connects to a cortical top-down circuit from motor to sensory cortices which controls accurate perception and memory of texture information, as Murayama's group previously discovered. This tri-regional circuit strengthens emotionally linked perceptual memories. Brain recordings showed that these three brain regions were cooperatively activated in the learning periods and reactivated during early non-REM sleep, but not during REM sleep, to reinforce the memory.

To test the importance of the non-REM reactivation, the researchers temporarily prevented amygdala inputs to the motor cortex that sends top-down inputs to the sensory area. When they did this during non-REM sleep, mice failed to retain the texture memory until day 5 despite the emotional experience. In contrast, blocking the signals during REM sleep had no effect, confirming that non-REM sleep is the key sleep stage for strengthening perceptual memories.

"Traditionally, REM sleep has been thought to be the primary stage for emotional memory processing," says Murayama. "Our findings challenge this idea and instead support non-REM sleep as the critical stage."

This study not only explains how emotion enhances other types of memories (e.g., perception) but also points to potential treatments for conditions like addiction in which symptoms are sometimes triggered by perceptual information in a phenomenon called flashback. Such information is thought to be strongly memorized in connection with emotional events that occurred even long before the episodes. By modulating brain activity in the amygdala and related regions during non-REM sleep to weaken perceptual memories that triggers flashbacks, doctors could potentially prevent or treat addiction.

Future research will explore how these findings apply to disease models, such as age-related memory decline or addiction. "For instance, it will be important to examine whether we can recover or even strengthen memories in aged-mice," says Murayama. "Our ultimate goal is to use this knowledge to develop treatments that improve mental health and memory-related conditions."


Story Source:

Materials provided by RIKEN. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Yoshihito Saito, Yuma Osako, Maya Odagawa, Yasuhiro Oisi, Chie Matsubara, Shigeki Kato, Kazuto Kobayashi, Mitsuhiro Morita, Joshua P. Johansen, Masanori Murayama. Amygdalo-cortical dialogue underlies memory enhancement by emotional association. Neuron, 2025; DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2025.01.001

Cite This Page:

RIKEN. "Positive emotions plus deep sleep equals longer-lasting perceptual memories." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 3 February 2025. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2025/02/250203203355.htm>.
RIKEN. (2025, February 3). Positive emotions plus deep sleep equals longer-lasting perceptual memories. ScienceDaily. Retrieved February 4, 2025 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2025/02/250203203355.htm
RIKEN. "Positive emotions plus deep sleep equals longer-lasting perceptual memories." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2025/02/250203203355.htm (accessed February 4, 2025).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES