How'd we get so picky about friendship late in life? Ask the chimps
New study shows evidence of nonhuman animals selecting who they socialize during aging
- Date:
- October 22, 2020
- Source:
- Harvard University
- Summary:
- When humans age, they tend to favor small circles of meaningful, already established friendships rather than seek new ones. People are also more likely to lean toward positive relationships rather than ones that bring tension or conflict. These behaviors were thought to be unique to humans but it turns out chimpanzees, one of our closest living relatives, have these traits, too. The study shows what's believed to be the first evidence of nonhuman animals actively selecting who they socialize with during aging.
- Share:
No new friends and no drama.
When humans age, they tend to favor small circles of meaningful, already established friendships rather than seek new ones. People are also more likely to lean toward positive relationships rather than ones that bring tension or conflict. These behaviors were thought to be unique to humans but it turns out chimpanzees, one of our closest living relatives, have these traits, too. Understanding why can help scientists gain a better picture of what healthy aging should look like and what triggers this social change.
The work is described in the journal Science and is authored by a team of psychologists and primatologists, including current and former researchers from the Harvard Department of Human Evolutionary Biology.
The study draws on 78,000 hours of observations, made between 1995 and 2016, which looked at the social interactions of 21 male chimpanzees between the ages of 15 and 58 years old in the Kibale National Park in Uganda. It shows what's believed to be the first evidence of nonhuman animals actively selecting who they socialize with during aging.
The researchers looked only at male chimpanzees because they show stronger social bonds and have more frequent social interactions than female chimps.
Analyzing a trove of data, the researchers saw that the chimpanzees displayed much of the same behavior aging humans exhibit. The older chimpanzees they studied, for instance, preferred spending more time with -- and grooming -- chimps they'd developed mutual friendships with over the years while younger chimps had more one-sided relationships where grooming wasn't always returned. Older males were also more likely to spend more time alone but interacted with more important social partners, like their aging mutual friends. And like older humans looking for some peace and quiet, the chimpanzees also showed a shift from negative interactions to more positive ones as they reached their twilight years. The preference is known as a positivity bias.
"The really cool thing is that we found that chimpanzees are showing these patterns that mirror those of humans," said Alexandra Rosati '05, an assistant professor of psychology and anthropology at the University of Michigan and one of the paper's lead authors.
Through future research that can help determine if these behaviors constitute the normal or successful course that aging should take, she added. It can serve as a model or baseline.
"There's really a pressing need to understand the biology of aging," Rosati said. "More humans are living longer than in the past, which can change the dynamics of aging."
Rosati is a former assistant professor and visiting fellow in HEB department where the study originated. Other Harvard-connected authors on the paper include Zarin Machanda '04, A.M., '09, Ph.D. who's now an assistant professor at Tufts University, Melissa Emery Thompson '00, A.M, '05, Ph.D., who's now an associate professor at New Mexico University, Lindsey Hagberg '17, who's now a medical student at Washington University, and Richard W. Wrangham, Ruth B. Moore Professor of Biological Anthropology and founder and co-director of the Kibale Chimpanzee Project.
Machanda and Thompson worked in Wrangham's lab as graduate students and currently serve as co-directors for the Kibale project, which has other authors on the paper including Martin N. Muller, a former postdoctoral-fellow in HEB. The project started as Hagberg's undergraduate senior thesis.
The study tested the origins of humans prioritizing close, positive relationships during aging and if it's really triggered by a theory known as socioemotional selectivity. The notion suggests that the central process driving social selectivity during aging results from people becoming aware that their time is running out and wanting to make the best of that time.
The findings from the study suggest there is more to understand.
"Even though chimps are very smart, they do not understand they're going to die," Wrangham said. "Much more likely something else is going on in chimps to explain why their relationships become more positive as they get older, and then the question is what applies to chimps the same as what applies to humans."
Some of the observations that led the researchers to their conclusions included looking at proximity and grooming habits. Older chimps preferred sitting close to those who preferred sitting close to them. These are categorized as mutual friendships while one-sided friendships are when one chimp prefers sitting close to another chimp but that other chimp doesn't share that habit.
Fifteen-year-old chimps had on average 2.1 one-sided friendships and 0.9 mutual friends while 40-year-old chimps almost didn't bother with one-side friendships (their average was .6), but did have plenty of mutual friends, an average of three. By looking at grooming habits, the researchers then saw the older chimps devote more energy into their relationships with mutual friends.
"We see individuals having these more lopsided friendships and then as they age they start really spending time with individuals that reciprocate," said Machanda, who was the paper's other lead author. "When you have this kind of mutual friendship, you actually groom that individual more, so these older chimps have these mutual friendships and they're actually grooming those individuals quite a bit. They're really invested in these relationships."
The scientists weren't entirely surprised by their findings. Part of it is because chimpanzees and humans are already a lot alike in terms of social organization and social choices. After all, chimpanzees, along with bonobos, share 99 percent of their DNA with humans.
"It raises the possibility that we are seeing behavioral systems that have been shared evolutionarily back to our common ancestor, around seven or eight million years ago," Wranham said.
Story Source:
Materials provided by Harvard University. Original written by Juan Siliezar. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.
Journal Reference:
- Alexandra G. Rosati, Lindsey Hagberg, Drew K. Enigk, Emily Otali, Melissa Emery Thompson, Martin N. Muller, Richard W. Wrangham, Zarin P. Machanda. Social selectivity in aging wild chimpanzees. Science, 2020; 370 (6515): 473 DOI: 10.1126/science.aaz9129
Cite This Page: