Brain mechanism involved in language learning
- Date:
- March 15, 2018
- Source:
- University of Sussex
- Summary:
- Psychologists found that when we learn the names of unfamiliar objects, brain regions involved in learning actively predict the objects the names correspond to.
- Share:
Learning a new language may be more of a science than an art, a University of Sussex study finds.
Psychologists found that when we learn the names of unfamiliar objects, brain regions involved in learning actively predict the objects the names correspond to. The brain tests these predictions just as scientists would test a scientific theory.
The team found that the hippocampus -- a brain region that is affected in Alzheimer's disease and some developmental language disorders -- plays a key role learning the names of objects via a "propose-but-verify" strategy. Using this strategy learners actively predict which of the words they hear correspond to each of the objects they see.
Twenty-three adults looked at scenes with multiple objects whilst listening to words in an MRI scanner. The MRI scanner allows psychologists to see which brain regions are active while participants carry out tests of memory and attention. The words and objects were made-up so that they were completely new to the study participants. Because multiple unknown words and objects were presented simultaneously, it was never immediately obvious which words corresponded to which object. The correspondences could only be learned across several minutes. However, by covertly proposing name-object correspondences and testing those proposals across many scenes, all of the adults were able to learn the words for all 18 previously unfamiliar objects. The MRI scans revealed that the hippocampus was central to this propose-but-verify mechanism. Specifically, it helped adults remember the word object correspondences over time.
The findings, published today, Thursday 15 March 2018, in the journal Current Biology, shed light on how the brain supports language acquisition. The findings have implications for both language education and our understanding of what is happening in languages disorders.
Lead researcher Dr Sam Berens said: "Children have a remarkable ability to learn new languages and it is hotly debated whether they use a propose-but-verify strategy during early language development. Our experiment shows that the hippocampus can support propose-but-verify learning in adults and that this learning mechanism is favoured over other strategies."
"A logical next step would be to apply the research techniques that we used in this study to investigate language impairments in children and adults. Children are able to learn new languages effortlessly, but it is still unclear if they learn words in the same way as adults. This research technique would give us more insight into studying language development and will leave us better equipped to help those with developmental language difficulties."
Senior researcher Dr Chris Bird, who oversaw the research project, is continuing to investigate the ways in which language learning is affected in Alzheimer's disease and whether some learning strategies are less affected by the condition than others. This programme of research is funded by the European Research Council.
Story Source:
Materials provided by University of Sussex. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.
Journal Reference:
- Sam C Berens, Jessica S Horst and Chris M Bird. Cross-situational learning is supported by propose-but-verify hypothesis testing. Current Biology, 2018 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.042
Cite This Page: