New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Students may forget relevant information in order to protect their own psyches

Study suggests people often don't recall memories that threaten the way they want to see themselves

Date:
April 20, 2017
Source:
University of California - Los Angeles
Summary:
University students who identify as strong in mathematics quickly forgot much of the material from a mathematics course. The explanation: We tend to forget unpleasant experiences and memories that threaten our self-image as a way to preserve our psychological well-being.
Share:
FULL STORY

UCLA-led research has found that students in a college mathematics course experienced a phenomenon similar to repression, the psychological process in which people forget emotional or traumatic events to protect themselves.

In a study published online by the Journal of Educational Psychology, the researchers found that the students who forgot the most content from the class were those who reported a high level of stress during the course. But, paradoxically, the study also found that the strong relationship between stress level and the tendency to forget course material was most prevalent among the students who are most confident in their own mathematical abilities.

The phenomenon, which the authors call "motivated forgetting," may occur because students are subconsciously protecting their own self-image as excellent mathematicians, said Gerardo Ramirez, a UCLA assistant professor of psychology and the study's lead author.

For the study, researchers analyzed 117 undergraduates in an advanced calculus course at UCLA. The students generally consider themselves to be strong in mathematics and plan to pursue careers that rely on high-level mathematical skills, so the logical assumption would be that they would be likely to remember most of the material from the course.

Researchers asked students a series of questions at the start of the course, including having them assess to what extent they see themselves as "math people." Each week throughout the course, students were asked to gauge how stressful they thought the course was. Then, the study's authors examined students' performance on the course's final exam and on another similar test two weeks later. On average, students' grades were 21 percent lower on the follow-up.

Among students who strongly considered themselves to be "math people," those who experienced a lot of stress performed measurably worse on the follow-up exam than those whose stress levels were lower.

The results were striking because, in the cases of the students whose stress levels were highest, test scores dropped by as much as a full letter grade -- from an A-minus to a B-minus, for example. But, according to Ramirez, the findings make sense from a psychological perspective.

"Students who found the course very stressful and difficult might have given in to the motivation to forget as a way to protect their identity as being good at math," he said. "We tend to forget unpleasant experiences and memories that threaten our self-image as a way to preserve our psychological well-being. And 'math people' whose identity is threatened by their previous stressful course experience may actively work to forget what they learned."

The idea that people are motivated to forget unpleasant experiences -- activating a sort of "psychological immune system" -- goes back to Sigmund and Anna Freud, the pioneers of psychoanalysis, Ramirez said.

The students who think of themselves as excellent at math and felt high levels of stress were also more likely to report they avoided thinking about the course after it ended more than other students did.

Previous studies by other researchers seem to support the concept of motivated forgetting. For example, a 2011 Harvard University study found that when people were asked to memorize an "honor code" and then pay themselves for solving a series of problems, those who cheated and overpaid themselves remembered less of the honor code at the end of the experiment than those who did not cheat.

"Motivated forgetting, or giving in to the desire to forget what we find threatening, is a defense mechanism people use against threats to the way they like to depict themselves," Ramirez said. "The students are highly motivated to do well and can't escape during the course, but as soon as they take their final exam, they can give in their desire to forget and try to suppress the information."

Ramirez said there are steps teachers can take to help students retain information. Some of them:

  • Emphasize the material's real-world applications. This will give students incentives to remember information and review it later on. "I think we often do a poor job of showing students why the content is relevant to their lives and future job skills," Ramirez said.
  • Cover the entire course in final exams. And not just the most recent material. "Non-cumulative exams tell students they can forget what they have already been tested on," he said.
  • Guard against learning-by-photo. Specifically, Ramirez advises students not to try to capture course notes by taking photos with their smartphones -- it might subtly create an impression that they don't need to actually learn the information.
  • Embrace the challenges. When his students struggle, Ramirez tells them the challenge they're facing will lead to deeper learning. "I try to change what 'struggle' means for them so that they don't feel threatened when they are stressed out about the material," he said.

Story Source:

Materials provided by University of California - Los Angeles. Original written by Stuart Wolpert. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Gerardo Ramirez, Ian M. McDonough, Ling Jin. Classroom Stress Promotes Motivated Forgetting of Mathematics Knowledge.. Journal of Educational Psychology, 2017; DOI: 10.1037/edu0000170

Cite This Page:

University of California - Los Angeles. "Students may forget relevant information in order to protect their own psyches." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 20 April 2017. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/04/170420090300.htm>.
University of California - Los Angeles. (2017, April 20). Students may forget relevant information in order to protect their own psyches. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 21, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/04/170420090300.htm
University of California - Los Angeles. "Students may forget relevant information in order to protect their own psyches." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/04/170420090300.htm (accessed December 21, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES