New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

The sound of status: People know high-power voices when they hear them

Date:
November 24, 2014
Source:
Association for Psychological Science
Summary:
Being in a position of power can fundamentally change the way you speak, altering basic acoustic properties of the voice, and other people are able to pick up on these vocal cues to know who is really in charge, according to new research.
Share:
FULL STORY

Being in a position of power can fundamentally change the way you speak, altering basic acoustic properties of the voice, and other people are able to pick up on these vocal cues to know who is really in charge, according to new research published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

We tend to focus on our words when we want to come across as powerful to others, but these findings suggest that basic acoustic cues also play an important role:

"Our findings suggest that whether it's parents attempting to assert authority over unruly children, haggling between a car salesman and customer, or negotiations between heads of states, the sound of the voices involved may profoundly determine the outcome of those interactions," says psychological scientist and lead researcher Sei Jin Ko of San Diego State University.

The researchers had long been interested in non-language-related properties of speech, but it was former UK prime minister Margaret Thatcher that inspired them to investigate the relationship between acoustic cues and power.

"It was quite well known that Thatcher had gone through extensive voice coaching to exude a more authoritative, powerful persona," explains Ko. "We wanted to explore how something so fundamental as power might elicit changes in the way a voice sounds, and how these situational vocal changes impact the way listeners perceive and behave toward the speakers."

Ko, along with Melody Sadler of San Diego State and Adam Galinsky of Columbia Business School, designed two studies to find out.

In the first experiment, they recorded 161 college students reading a passage aloud; this first recording captured baseline acoustics. The participants were then randomly assigned them to play a specific role in an ensuing negotiation exercise.

Students assigned to a "high" rank were told to go into the negotiation imagining that they either had a strong alternative offer, valuable inside information, or high status in the workplace, or they were asked to recall an experience in which they had power before the negotiation started. Low-rank students, on the other hand, were told to imagine they had either a weak offer, no inside information, or low workplace status, or they were asked to recall an experience in which they lacked power.

The students then read a second passage aloud, as if they were leading off negotiations with their imaginary adversary, and their voices were recorded. Everyone read the same opening, allowing the researchers to examine acoustics while holding the speech content constant across all participants.

Comparing the first and second recordings, the researchers found that the voices of students assigned to high-power roles tended to go up in pitch, become more monotone (less variable in pitch), and become more variable in loudness than the voices of students assigned low-power roles.

"Amazingly, power affected our participants' voices in almost the exact same way that Thatcher's voice changed after her vocal training," says Galinsky.

And the students' vocal cues didn't go unnoticed. A second experiment with a separate group of college students revealed that listeners, who had no knowledge of the first experiment, were able to pick up on these power-related vocal cues to determine who did and did not have power: Listeners ranked speakers who had been assigned to the high-rank group as more likely to engage in high-power behaviors, and they were able to categorize whether a speaker had high or low rank with considerable accuracy.

In line with the vocal changes observed in the first experiments, listeners tended to associate higher pitch and voices that varied in loudness with high-power behaviors. They also associated louder voices with higher power.

"These findings suggest that listeners are quite perceptive to these subtle variations in vocal cues and they use these cues to decide who is in charge," says Galinsky.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Association for Psychological Science. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. S. J. Ko, M. S. Sadler, A. D. Galinsky. The Sound of Power: Conveying and Detecting Hierarchical Rank Through Voice. Psychological Science, 2014; DOI: 10.1177/0956797614553009

Cite This Page:

Association for Psychological Science. "The sound of status: People know high-power voices when they hear them." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 24 November 2014. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/11/141124081040.htm>.
Association for Psychological Science. (2014, November 24). The sound of status: People know high-power voices when they hear them. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 26, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/11/141124081040.htm
Association for Psychological Science. "The sound of status: People know high-power voices when they hear them." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/11/141124081040.htm (accessed December 26, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES