New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Early vocabulary size is genetically linked to ADHD, literacy, and cognition

Date:
March 1, 2024
Source:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
Summary:
Are genetic factors underlying children's language development linked to later-life outcomes? In a genome-wide analysis, an international research team found genetic associations between children's early vocabulary size and later-life ADHD, literacy, and general cognition. These associations changed dynamically across the first three years of life. Both producing more words in infancy and understanding fewer words in toddlerhood were associated with a higher risk of ADHD.
Share:
FULL STORY

Early language development is an important predictor of children's later language, reading and learning skills. Moreover, language learning difficulties are related to neurodevelopmental conditions such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).

Children typically start to utter their first words between 10 and 15 months of age. At around two years of age, they may produce between 100 -- 600 words, and understand many more. Each child embarks on its own developmental path of language learning, resulting in large individual differences. "Some variation in language development can be related to variation in the genetic code stored in our cells," says senior researcher Beate St Pourcain, lead scientist on the study.

Word production and understanding

To understand how genetics plays a role in the development of children's word production and understanding, the team carried out a genome-wide meta-analysis study (GWAS) of infant (15-18 months) and toddler (24-38 months) vocabulary size. In early measures of vocabulary size, parents report which words their children say and/or understand from a given word list.

The team used vocabulary and genetic data from 17,298 English-, Danish- or Dutch-speaking children. The number of spoken words was available for both infants and toddlers. The number of understood words was only available for toddlers. Later-life outcomes were mostly studied with genetic summary information from large independent consortia. These included literacy (spelling, reading, and phoneme awareness), cognition (general intelligence and number of years of education) and neurodevelopmental conditions (genetic risk of ADHD and ASD, as well as directly observed ADHD-related symptoms in some of the studied children).

"Learning to speak" and "Speaking to learn"

The researchers identified multiple genetic factors underlying vocabulary size in infancy and toddlerhood. Consistently, genetic associations with later-life literacy, cognition and ADHD-related measures varied during development. Both infant and toddler word production were related to literacy abilities such as spelling, but associations with general cognition were only found for toddler vocabulary scores. Toddlers have mastered some language fluency and may "speak to learn," involving higher-level cognitive processing, while the development of verbal abiltities may start earlier.

The team also found that in infancy, a larger number of spoken words was genetically associated with both an increased risk for ADHD and more ADHD symptoms. However, this genetic relationship was reversed in toddlerhood: there, a smaller number of understood words was associated with more ADHD symptoms. It is possible that in infancy, when children are "learning to speak," the number of spoken words captures speech-related processes. Also, children with a higher genetic risk of ADHD may be inclined to express themselves more. In contrast, during the phase of "speaking to learn" when vocabulary size is linked to cognition, higher genetic ADHD risk may be associated with lower verbal and cognitive abilities.

According to St Pourcain, "Genetic influences underlying vocabulary size rapidly change across less than two years during infancy and toddlerhood. Adopting a developmental perspective, our findings provide a better understanding of early speech- and language-related aetiological processes in health and disorder." First author Ellen Verhoef adds: "This research indicates the relevance of vocabulary size assessed during the first few years in life for future behaviour and cognition, emphasising the need for more data collection efforts during infancy and toddlerhood."

Early language development is an important predictor of children's later language, reading and learning skills. Moreover, language learning difficulties are related to neurodevelopmental conditions such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).

Children typically start to utter their first words between 10 and 15 months of age. At around two years of age, they may produce between 100 -- 600 words, and understand many more. Each child embarks on its own developmental path of language learning, resulting in large individual differences. "Some variation in language development can be related to variation in the genetic code stored in our cells," says senior researcher Beate St Pourcain, lead scientist on the study.

Word production and understanding

To understand how genetics plays a role in the development of children's word production and understanding, the team carried out a genome-wide meta-analysis study (GWAS) of infant (15-18 months) and toddler (24-38 months) vocabulary size. In early measures of vocabulary size, parents report which words their children say and/or understand from a given word list.

The team used vocabulary and genetic data from 17,298 English-, Danish- or Dutch-speaking children. The number of spoken words was available for both infants and toddlers. The number of understood words was only available for toddlers. Later-life outcomes were mostly studied with genetic summary information from large independent consortia. These included literacy (spelling, reading, and phoneme awareness), cognition (general intelligence and number of years of education) and neurodevelopmental conditions (genetic risk of ADHD and ASD, as well as directly observed ADHD-related symptoms in some of the studied children).

"Learning to speak" and "Speaking to learn"

The researchers identified multiple genetic factors underlying vocabulary size in infancy and toddlerhood. Consistently, genetic associations with later-life literacy, cognition and ADHD-related measures varied during development. Both infant and toddler word production were related to literacy abilities such as spelling, but associations with general cognition were only found for toddler vocabulary scores. Toddlers have mastered some language fluency and may "speak to learn," involving higher-level cognitive processing, while the development of verbal abiltities may start earlier.

The team al

The team used vocabulary and genetic data from 17,298 English-, Danish- or Dutch-speaking children. The number of spoken words was available for both infants and toddlers. The number of understood words was only available for toddlers. Later-life outcomes were mostly studied with genetic summary information from large independent consortia. These included literacy (spelling, reading, and phoneme awareness), cognition (general intelligence and number of years of education) and neurodevelopmental conditions (genetic risk of ADHD and ASD, as well as directly observed ADHD-related symptoms in some of the studied children).

"Learning to speak" and "Speaking to learn"

The researchers identified multiple genetic factors underlying vocabulary size in infancy and toddlerhood. Consistently, genetic associations with later-life literacy, cognition and ADHD-related measures varied during development. Both infant and toddler word production were related to literacy abilities such as spelling, but associations with general cognition were only found for toddler vocabulary scores. Toddlers have mastered some language fluency and may "speak to learn," involving higher-level cognitive processing, while the development of verbal abiltities may start earlier.

The team also found that in infancy, a larger number of spoken words was genetically associated with both an increased risk for ADHD and more ADHD symptoms. However, this genetic relationship was reversed in toddlerhood: there, a smaller number of understood words was associated with more ADHD symptoms. It is possible that in infancy, when children are "learning to speak," the number of spoken words captures speech-related processes. Also, children with a higher genetic risk of ADHD may be inclined to express themselves more. In contrast, during the phase of "speaking to learn" when vocabulary size is linked to cognition, higher genetic ADHD risk may be associated with lower verbal and cognitive abilities.

According to St Pourcain, "Genetic influences underlying vocabulary size rapidly change across less than two years during infancy and toddlerhood. Adopting a developmental perspective, our findings provide a better understanding of early speech- and language-related aetiological processes in health and disorder." First author Ellen Verhoef adds: "This research indicates the relevance of vocabulary size assessed during the first few years in life for future behaviour and cognition, emphasising the need for more data collection efforts during infancy and toddlerhood."

Early language development is an important predictor of children's later language, reading and learning skills. Moreover, language learning difficulties are related to neurodevelopmental conditions such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).

Children typically start to utter their first words between 10 and 15 months of age. At around two years of age, they may produce between 100 -- 600 words, and understand many more. Each child embarks on its own developmental path of language learning, resulting in large individual differences. "Some variation in language development can be related to variation in the genetic code stored in our cells," says senior researcher Beate St Pourcain, lead scientist on the study.

Word production and understanding

To understand how genetics plays a role in the development of children's word production and understanding, the team carried out a genome-wide meta-analysis study (GWAS) of infant (15-18 months) and toddler (24-38 months) vocabulary size. In early measures of vocabulary size, parents report which words their children say and/or understand from a given word list.

The team used vocabulary and genetic data from 17,298 English-, Danish- or Dutch-speaking children. The number of spoken words was available for both infants and toddlers. The number of understood words was only available for toddlers. Later-life outcomes were mostly studied with genetic summary information from large independent consortia. These included literacy (spelling, reading, and phoneme awareness), cognition (general intelligence and number of years of education) and neurodevelopmental conditions (genetic risk of ADHD and ASD, as well as directly observed ADHD-related symptoms in some of the studied children).

"Learning to speak" and "Speaking to learn"

The researchers identified multiple genetic factors underlying vocabulary size in infancy and toddlerhood. Consistently, genetic associations with later-life literacy, cognition and ADHD-related measures varied during development. Both infant and toddler word production were related to literacy abilities such as spelling, but associations with general cognition were only found for toddler vocabulary scores. Toddlers have mastered some language fluency and may "speak to learn," involving higher-level cognitive processing, while the development of verbal abiltities may start earlier.

The team also found that in infancy, a larger number of spoken words was genetically associated with both an increased risk for ADHD and more ADHD symptoms. However, this genetic relationship was reversed in toddlerhood: there, a smaller number of understood words was associated with more ADHD symptoms. It is possible that in infancy, when children are "learning to speak," the number of spoken words captures speech-related processes. Also, children with a higher genetic risk of ADHD may be inclined to express themselves more. In contrast, during the phase of "speaking to learn" when vocabulary size is linked to cognition, higher genetic ADHD risk may be associated with lower verbal and cognitive abilities.

According to St Pourcain, "Genetic influences underlying vocabulary size rapidly change across less than two years during infancy and toddlerhood. Adopting a developmental perspective, our findings provide a better understanding of early speech- and language-related aetiological processes in health and disorder." First author Ellen Verhoef adds: "This research indicates the relevance of vocabulary size assessed during the first few years in life for future behaviour and cognition, emphasising the need for more data collection efforts during infancy and toddlerhood."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Ellen Verhoef, Andrea G. Allegrini, Philip R. Jansen, Katherine Lange, Carol A. Wang, Angela T. Morgan, Tarunveer S. Ahluwalia, Christos Symeonides, Else Eising, Marie-Christine Franken, Elina Hypponen, Toby Mansell, Mitchell Olislagers, Emina Omerovic, Kaili Rimfeld, Fenja Schlag, Saskia Selzam, Chin Yang Shapland, Henning Tiemeier, Andrew J.O. Whitehouse, Richard Saffery, Klaus Bønnelykke, Sheena Reilly, Craig E. Pennell, Melissa Wake, Charlotte A.M. Cecil, Robert Plomin, Simon E. Fisher, Beate St. Pourcain, Ole A. Andreassen, Meike Bartels, Dorret Boomsma, Philip S. Dale, Erik Ehli, Dietmar Fernandez-Orth, Mònica Guxens, Christian Hakulinen, Kathleen Mullan Harris, Simon Haworth, Lucía de Hoyos, Vincent Jaddoe, Liisa Keltikangas-Järvinen, Terho Lehtimäki, Christel Middeldorp, Josine L. Min, Pashupati P. Mishra, Pål Rasmus Njølstad, Jordi Sunyer, Ashley E. Tate, Nicholas Timpson, Camiel van der Laan, Martine Vrijheid, Eero Vuoksimaa, Alyce Whipp, Eivind Ystrom, ACTION Consortium, Barwon Infant Study investigator group. Genome-wide Analyses of Vocabulary Size in Infancy and Toddlerhood: Associations With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Literacy, and Cognition-Related Traits. Biological Psychiatry, 2023; DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2023.11.025

Cite This Page:

Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. "Early vocabulary size is genetically linked to ADHD, literacy, and cognition." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 1 March 2024. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2024/03/240301134657.htm>.
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. (2024, March 1). Early vocabulary size is genetically linked to ADHD, literacy, and cognition. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 20, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2024/03/240301134657.htm
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. "Early vocabulary size is genetically linked to ADHD, literacy, and cognition." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2024/03/240301134657.htm (accessed December 20, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES