New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Human brain takes stock of blame

Brain separates outcomes when human error is the cause

Date:
November 9, 2023
Source:
University of Iowa
Summary:
Researchers have found that the human brain can distinguish between an outcome caused by human error and one in which the person's decision-making is blameless. The brain takes just one second to separate the outcomes, and in cases involving human error, extra time to catalog the mistake.
Share:
FULL STORY

The human mind does not like to make mistakes -- and makes time to avoid repeating them.

A new study from University of Iowa researchers shows how the human brain, in just one second, can distinguish between an outcome caused by human error and one in which the person is not directly to blame.

Moreover, the researchers found, in cases of human error, the brain takes additional time to catalog the error and inform the rest of the body about it to avoid repeating the miscue.

"The novel aspect about this study is the brain can very quickly distinguish whether an undesirable outcome is due to a (human) error, or due to something else," says Jan Wessel, professor in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Iowa and the study's corresponding author. "If the brain realizes an error was the cause, it will then start additional processes to avoid further errors, which it won't do if the outcome wasn't due to its own action."

The Iowa researchers learned about the brain's ability to separate human error from a non-self-inflicted error by asking 76 young adults to look at a cluster of arrows and choose the correct direction one specific arrow was pointing. Nearly every time the subjects responded -- almost always correctly, given the task's simplicity -- a triangle would appear on the screen. But every now and then, another symbol (an anchor, frog, helicopter, etc.) would appear on screen, meant to mimic a "surprise" or unexpected outcome, and, importantly, appearing even when the subject responded correctly and expected the triangle symbol.

The researchers measured at three different intervals (350, 1,700, and 3,000 milliseconds) how the brain responded to situations with the standard outcome (the triangle) and the surprise outcome (a different symbol).

What they found is that the brain can distinguish between the two outcomes after about one second (1,000 milliseconds).

If human error is the reason for the outcome, the brain remains active for an additional two to three seconds, the researchers found. That means the brain realizes a mistake was made, and essentially wants to learn from it.

"When it is something that has to do with my own action and I can do something about it, then the brain takes a few seconds to reconfigure the entire cognitive apparatus, the visual system, the motor system," says Wessel, who has a joint appointment in the Department of Neurology. "It's as if the brain is taking a moment to fill in the rest of the body, the senses, the motor control, to tell the other working parts, 'Let's not do this again.'"

The researchers also measured brain waves through scalp electroencephalograms (EEGs) and observed ongoing neural activity that was unique to instances when human error occurred.

"Indeed, we found that while both errors and unexpected outcomes of correct actions led to comparable neural activity early on, only errors showed reliable, sustained brain activity more than a second after the response," Wessel says.

Previous research had shown the brain can recognize instances when human error has occurred, but there was debate about whether the brain's reaction to an outcome was the same regardless of whether the cause was human error or not.

"Some have argued that we don't actually have a genuine error detection system in the brain," Wessel notes.

But Wessel's research demonstrates that the brain does make an error/no error distinction and communicates information related to either outcome with the rest of the body.

"All in all, this shows that we do have genuine, error-specific systems in the human brain that detect our action errors that trigger adaptive responses, such as the strategic slowing of ongoing actions," Wessel says.

The study, "Early action error processing is due to domain-general surprise while later processing is error-specific," was published online Oct. 13 in JNeurosci, a journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

The study's first author is Yoojeong Choo, a graduate student in Wessel's lab. A co-author is Alec Mather, an Iowa graduate who worked in Wessel's lab as an undergraduate and is now data science manager at Sony Music Entertainment.

The National Institutes of Health funded the research.


Story Source:

Materials provided by University of Iowa. Original written by Richard C. Lewis. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Yoojeong Choo, Alec Mather, Jan R. Wessel. Early Action Error Processing Is Due to Domain-General Surprise, Whereas Later Processing Is Error Specific. The Journal of Neuroscience, 2023; 43 (45): 7678 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1334-23.2023

Cite This Page:

University of Iowa. "Human brain takes stock of blame." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 9 November 2023. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2023/11/231109121458.htm>.
University of Iowa. (2023, November 9). Human brain takes stock of blame. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 20, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2023/11/231109121458.htm
University of Iowa. "Human brain takes stock of blame." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2023/11/231109121458.htm (accessed December 20, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES