New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Change the bias, change the behavior? Maybe not

Date:
August 2, 2019
Source:
Washington University in St. Louis
Summary:
In a meta-analysis of published research, psychologists tease out how changes in implicit bias do -- and do not -- appear to lead to changes in behavior. And why that might be.
Share:
FULL STORY

The concept of implicit bias has made its way into the general consciousness, most often in the context of racial bias. More broadly, however, implicit biases can affect how people think of anything -- from their thoughts about cookies to those about white men.

"All the little ways in which our everyday thinking about social stuff is unconscious or uncontrollable," wrote Calvin Lai, assistant professor of psychology in Arts & Sciences at Washington University in St. Louis, in an article in DCist. "The stuff that we don't realize is influencing us when we make decisions."

Along with a broader cultural awareness of implicit bias is idea that the actions that they influence can be changed by eliminating the bias itself.

Change the bias, changes in the behavior will follow. It seems logical enough.

If true, reducing implicit bias could be put to practical use for anything from ending discrimination (removing a bias in favor of white males) to losing weight (dialing down a cookie bias).

In a meta-analysis of research papers published on the subject of implicit bias, however, Lai found that the evidence does not show this kind of causal relationship.

The research is published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Lai worked with Patrick Forscher, of the University of Arkansas, to systematically review 492 studies that dealt with changing people's "automatic mental processes," the uncontrollable, unconscious mental processes that have come to be known in particular contexts as "implicit bias."

The studies contained more than 87,000 participants. After crunching the numbers, Lai and Forscher saw that studies suggest biases can, in fact, be changed -- although not dramatically.

When they honed in, looking at 63 studies that explicitly considered a link between changes in bias and changes in actions, however, they found no evidence of a causal relationship.

"We definitely didn't expect this," Lai said. "And it challenges assumptions about the relationship between implicit bias and behavior."

Lai suggested four possible reasons that a link was not established in the meta-analysis:

Measurement errors: The way outcomes were measured may have picked up on changes unrelated to the underlying bias. For example, Lai said, such a measurement would be analogous to "moving the mercury around within a thermometer rather than changing the heat in the room."

Confounds: After tests to measure an implicit bias, something happened, unrelated to the changed subjects' behavior.

Measured too narrow of a bias: Appeared to assess the same associations, but maybe the effects were too broad to capture a change associated with the change in bias. For example, the implicit bias measured was about broad attitudes toward White vs. Black people, but the behaviors measured were about behavior toward a specific person of a particular race. In that case, the attitude measured may have been too general.

No causal relationship: Implicit bias doesn't affect behavior at all.

This last option doesn't sit well with Lai. "It would open a theoretical can of worms because there are decades of experiments in other lines of research showing evaluation without conscious intention or control," he said.

However, Lai said there is a more effective way to change these behaviors; one that doesn't rely on changing people's implicit biases: ridding society of the features that cause people to act in a biased way.

For example, reducing subjectivity makes it more difficult for a person's biases to affect decision-making. Instead of relying on a "gut feeling" for a hiring decision, for example, lay out the requirements first, and stick to them.

Or, in the cookie realm, don't have any on hand -- not at home or at the office -- and don't drive past the bakery on the way home.

On an individual level, Lai said, "Equip people with strategies to resist the environment's biasing influence.

"The power of counterstereotypes is not to be underestimated," Lai wrote in a paper describing possible ways to counteract implicit biases. "And if counterstereotypical encounters become typical, shift in attitudes and beliefs will follow."

Lai points out that this study was heavily constrained by the available literature. The studies they included focused on brief interventions and assessments and was heavily skewed toward a certain demographic: university students.

"At this time, we can't distinguish between these potential explanations for the results," Lai said. "Carefully controlled studies need to be conducted to rule out -- or rule in -- these explanations."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Washington University in St. Louis. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Patrick S. Forscher, Calvin K. Lai, Jordan R. Axt, Charles R. Ebersole, Michelle Herman, Patricia G. Devine, Brian A. Nosek. A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2019; DOI: 10.1037/pspa0000160

Cite This Page:

Washington University in St. Louis. "Change the bias, change the behavior? Maybe not." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 2 August 2019. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/08/190802144415.htm>.
Washington University in St. Louis. (2019, August 2). Change the bias, change the behavior? Maybe not. ScienceDaily. Retrieved November 20, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/08/190802144415.htm
Washington University in St. Louis. "Change the bias, change the behavior? Maybe not." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/08/190802144415.htm (accessed November 20, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES