New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Childhood poverty may have lasting effects on cognitive skills in old age

Date:
September 26, 2018
Source:
American Academy of Neurology
Summary:
Children who grow up in poverty or who are otherwise socially and economically disadvantaged may be more likely in old age to score lower than others on tests of cognitive skills, according to a new study.
Share:
FULL STORY

Children who grow up in poverty or who are otherwise socially and economically disadvantaged may be more likely in old age to score lower than others on tests of cognitive skills, according to a study published in the September 26, 2018, online issue of Neurology®, the medical journal of the American Academy of Neurology.

Cognitive skills include thinking, learning, reasoning, remembering and problem solving.

"Just like the body, the brain ages, but for some it may age faster than others," said study author Pavla ?ermáková, MD, PhD, of the Czech National Institute of Mental Health in Klecany, Czech Republic. "A growing body of evidence suggests aging of the brain may occur over a lifetime with its roots in childhood. Our study looked at a very large number of people from different backgrounds and geographic locations and found that social and economic disadvantages in childhood may indeed have a negative impact on cognitive skills."

For the study, researchers examined data on 20,244 people from 16 European countries who were part of a larger study called the Survey on Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe. The average age of participants chosen for this study was 71 at the start of the study. Participants were interviewed and tested once and then again at least one more time an average of five years later.

To determine cognitive skills, participants were given tests that measured verbal and memory skills, like being able to name animals, learning new words and recall them after a delay.

To determine socioeconomic hardship in childhood, participants were asked questions about their households at age 10, using a method known as a "life history calendar," a technique used to improve the accuracy of recalled information. Questions included the number of rooms, the number of people living in the home and the number of books in the home. Researchers calculated a ratio for the number of people in the home to the number of rooms and considered persons with the highest ratio and the lowest number of books as those having experienced socioeconomic hardship.

After examining this data, researchers determined that 4 percent of the entire group, or 844 people, experienced socioeconomic hardship in childhood. Researchers found that those who were socioeconomically disadvantaged scored lower in the cognitive tests. In addition, they were also less educated, were to a lower extent employed and less frequently had a partner in the home. They also scored higher in symptoms of depression, were less physically active and in general less healthy.

Researchers found for all participants, cognitive test scores ranged from -2.39 to 3.45 at the beginning of the study. These numbers represent a sum of z-standardized scores of each cognitive test and indicate how well the participants performed cognitively. Negative scores represent a lower level of cognitive performance.

After adjusting for age, sex and geographic location, researchers found those who experienced socioeconomic hardship in childhood performed lower on the cognitive tests than the rest of the group by an average of .27 points than those who didn't experience hardship. Even after taking into account differences in social and clinical factors such as education, employment, depression, body mass index, physical activity and cardiovascular diseases, they still scored an average .15 points lower.

While researchers found a difference between the two groups in cognitive skills, they found no link between socioeconomic hardship in childhood and a decline in these skills over time.

"While our research is observational, and cause and effect cannot be determined, it is by far the largest group of people ever studied on this topic," said ?ermáková. "Our study shows that the environment where we grew up is mirrored in the level of our cognitive skills when we are old; and this is only partly explained by education, depression or different life style factors. However, the childhood socioeconomic environment no longer affects how we fight against the declining skills while we age. We believe that the focus of strategies aiming to protect cognitive health should be shifted into childhood, taking into account that children facing social and economic challenges should be provided with more resources to counter the disadvantages they face."

A limitation of the study is that participants had to recall information from childhood and memories may not always be accurate. Also, those who face hardship in childhood also face a higher risk of death, so healthier people may have been overrepresented in this study.

The study was funded by the European Commission, the German Ministry of Education and Research, the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science and the U.S. National Institute on Aging.


Story Source:

Materials provided by American Academy of Neurology. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Pavla Cermakova, Tomas Formanek, Anna Kagstrom, Petr Winkler. Socioeconomic position in childhood and cognitive aging in Europe. Neurology, 2018; DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006390

Cite This Page:

American Academy of Neurology. "Childhood poverty may have lasting effects on cognitive skills in old age." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 26 September 2018. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/09/180926164039.htm>.
American Academy of Neurology. (2018, September 26). Childhood poverty may have lasting effects on cognitive skills in old age. ScienceDaily. Retrieved November 20, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/09/180926164039.htm
American Academy of Neurology. "Childhood poverty may have lasting effects on cognitive skills in old age." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/09/180926164039.htm (accessed November 20, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES