New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Do you hear what I hear?

Infants with autism risk may be less able to distinguish between familiar and unfamiliar speech patterns, study suggests

Date:
May 24, 2019
Source:
Columbia University Irving Medical Center
Summary:
A new study found that infants at high risk for autism were less attuned to differences in speech patterns than low-risk infants. The findings suggest that interventions to improve language skills should begin during infancy for those at high risk for autism.
Share:
FULL STORY

A new study by Columbia University researchers found that infants at high risk for autism were less attuned to differences in speech patterns than low-risk infants. The findings suggest that interventions to improve language skills should begin during infancy for those at high risk for autism.

The findings were published in Brain and Language.

"Humans are born with an astonishing ability to distinguish basic sound units that make up all of the world's languages," says Kristina Denisova, PhD, assistant professor of clinical psychology at the Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons. "But why some infants at high familial risk for autism spectrum disorder are less likely to develop their language-specific competence in toddlerhood has remained a mystery."

Previously, Denisova showed that high-risk infants (those who had a sibling with autism) were less likely to turn their heads in response to spoken language than typically developing infants. Denisova says that "our team dissociated between head movements in infants at high vs. low familial risk for developing autism and detected the signal of future atypical development as early as 1-2 months after birth."

A large body of research suggests that as an infant grows, future language development depends in part on the ability to distinguish sounds and elements of speech that are familiar vs. those that are novel -- including elements of pronunciation, such as stress patterns on different syllables. Sensitivity to specific stress patterns in one's language serve as important cues for learning language.

In the current study, the researchers studied 52 9- to 10-month-old infants who heard speech with familiar and unfamiliar stress patterns while undergoing MRI. Half of the infants were at high risk of autism. They recorded the head movements throughout the scan and studied whether features of head movements differ between the two cohorts.

Low-risk infants turned their heads more frequently while listening to speech with different syllabic patterns, while the high-risk infants did not. High-risk infants had significantly worse receptive language scores and the most atypical head-turning patterns on this task. Those 9- to 10-month-olds who had more abnormal head-turning behavior during three types of exposure (listening to alternating stress speech, listening to language, and during sleeping) were more likely to develop autism spectrum disorder by age three.

Denisova studied additional data sets in an attempt to explain what mechanisms might explain the difference in response. Her examination of studies of 774 infants confirmed that high-risk infants have lower receptive language scores compared to low-risk infants, further suggesting atypical processing of speech in the high-risk group. A final query compared vocal learning genes with convergent expression in humans and songbirds (zebra finches) in an important motor region for vocal learning circuitry (laryngeal motor cortex, LMC) against likely gene-disrupting mutations in autism genetics publications. This analysis revealed a potential candidate mechanism for disrupted vocal learning in humans, such that it may be less acquisitive.

"These findings regarding differences in early language processing in children at high familial risk for autism are intriguing and deserve replication and extension. Of great interest will be whether and how these early difference in language processing relate to the differences in social communication that are so key to understanding autism," says Agnes H. Whitaker, MD, clinical professor of psychiatry at the Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians and Surgeons.

The study is titled, "Failure to attune to language predicts autism in high risk infants."

Kristina Denisova, PhD was sole author.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Columbia University Irving Medical Center. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Kristina Denisova. Failure to attune to language predicts autism in high risk infants. Brain and Language, 2019; DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.04.002

Cite This Page:

Columbia University Irving Medical Center. "Do you hear what I hear?." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 24 May 2019. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/05/190524113533.htm>.
Columbia University Irving Medical Center. (2019, May 24). Do you hear what I hear?. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 3, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/05/190524113533.htm
Columbia University Irving Medical Center. "Do you hear what I hear?." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/05/190524113533.htm (accessed December 3, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES