New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Practice makes perfect: Switching between languages pays off

Bilingual toddlers who obtain more practice in language switching are better at certain types of problem solving

Date:
February 3, 2016
Source:
Concordia University
Summary:
Bilingual children are better than monolinguals at a certain type of mental control, and that those children with more practice switching between languages have even greater skills.
Share:
FULL STORY

It's estimated that half of the world's population speaks two or more languages. But are there hidden benefits to being bilingual? Research from Concordia reveals a new perk visible in the problem-solving skills of toddlers.

The results of a study recently published by the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology show that bilingual children are better than monolinguals at a certain type of mental control, and that those children with more practice switching between languages have even greater skills.

Bilingual speakers can thank the sometimes-arduous practice of switching from one language to another for this skill. "This switching becomes more frequent as children grow older and as their vocabulary size increases," says Diane Poulin-Dubois, a professor in Concordia's Department of Psychology and the study's senior author.

"Therefore, the superior performance on these conflict tasks appears to be due to bilinguals' strengthened cognitive flexibility and selective attention abilities as they have increased experience in switching across languages in expressive vocabulary."

Poulin-Dubois and Cristina Crivello, a graduate student with Concordia's Centre for Research in Human Development (CRDH), led a group of researchers* in a longitudinal investigation, which compared bilingual toddlers to their monolingual peers, tracking the tots as they gained greater vocabularies in each of their two languages.

For the study, the researchers assessed the vocabularies of 39 bilingual children and 43 monolinguals when they were aged 24 months, and then again at 31 months. During the second assessment, the researchers also had the young participants perform a battery of tasks to test their cognitive flexibility and memory skills.

"For the most part, there was no difference between the bilingual and monolingual toddlers," says Poulin-Dubois, who is also a member of CRDH. "But that changed dramatically when it came to the conflict inhibition test, and the differences were especially apparent in the bilingual toddlers whose vocabulary had increased most."

In this case, conflict inhibition refers to the mental process of overriding a well learned rule that you would normally pay attention to.

To assess toddlers' abilities in this domain, Crivello, who undertook the research as part of her master's thesis and is the first author of the study, administered two tests:

1. Reverse categorization -- participants were told to put a set of little blocks into a little bucket and big blocks into a big bucket. Then the instructions were switched -- big blocks in the little bucket and little blocks in the big bucket.

2. Shape conflict -- participants were shown pictures of different sized fruit and asked to name them. Then a new series of images was shown, with a small fruit embedded inside a large one. Toddlers were asked to point to the little fruit.

It wasn't surprising to the researchers that the bilingual children performed significantly better on the conflict inhibition tasks than did their monolingual counterparts.

"Language switching underlies the bilingual advantage on conflict tasks," says Crivello. "In conflict inhibition, the child has to ignore certain information -- the size of a block relative to a bucket, or the fact that one fruit is inside another. That mirrors the experience of having to switch between languages, using a second language even though the word from a first language might be more easily accessible."

The unique feature of the study was the finding that the more language switching toddlers engaged in, the more it benefitted them. Within the bilingual group of toddlers, those who had amassed a greater number of "doublets" -- pairs of words in each language, such as dog/chien -- performed even better on the conflict inhibition tasks.

"By the end of the third year of life, the average bilingual child uses two words for most concepts in his or her vocabulary, so young bilingual children gradually acquire more experience in switching between languages," says Poulin-Dubois.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Concordia University. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Cristina Crivello, Olivia Kuzyk, Monyka Rodrigues, Margaret Friend, Pascal Zesiger, Diane Poulin-Dubois. The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers’ executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2016; 141: 121 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004

Cite This Page:

Concordia University. "Practice makes perfect: Switching between languages pays off." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 3 February 2016. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/02/160203110936.htm>.
Concordia University. (2016, February 3). Practice makes perfect: Switching between languages pays off. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 3, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/02/160203110936.htm
Concordia University. "Practice makes perfect: Switching between languages pays off." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2016/02/160203110936.htm (accessed December 3, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES