What goes wrong in the brain when someone can't spell
- Date:
- February 2, 2016
- Source:
- Johns Hopkins University
- Summary:
- By studying stroke victims who have lost the ability to spell, researchers have pinpointed the parts of the brain that control how we write words.
- Share:
By studying stroke victims who have lost the ability to spell, researchers have pinpointed the parts of the brain that control how we write words.
In the latest issue of the journal Brain, Johns Hopkins University neuroscientists link basic spelling difficulties for the first time with damage to seemingly unrelated regions of the brain, shedding new light on the mechanics of language and memory.
"When something goes wrong with spelling, it's not one thing that always happens -- different things can happen and they come from different breakdowns in the brain's machinery," said lead author Brenda Rapp, a professor in the Department of Cognitive Sciences. "Depending on what part breaks, you'll have different symptoms."
Rapp's team studied 15 years' worth of cases in which 33 people were left with spelling impairments after suffering strokes. Some of the people had long-term memory difficulties, others working-memory issues.
With long-term memory difficulties, people can't remember how to spell words they once knew and tend to make educated guesses. They could probably correctly guess a predictably spelled word like "camp," but with a more unpredictable spelling like "sauce," they might try "soss." In severe cases, people trying to spell "lion" might offer things like "lonp," "lint" and even "tiger." With working memory issues, people know how to spell words but they have trouble choosing the correct letters or assembling the letters in the correct order -- "lion" might be "liot," "lin," "lino," or "liont."
The team used computer mapping to chart the brain lesions of each individual and found that in the long-term memory cases, damage appeared on two areas of the left hemisphere, one towards the front of the brain and the other at the lower part of the brain towards the back. In working memory cases, the lesions were primarily also in the left hemisphere but in a very different area in the upper part of the brain towards the back.
"I was surprised to see how distant and distinct the brain regions are that support these two subcomponents of the writing process, especially two subcomponents that are so closely inter-related during spelling that some have argued that they shouldn't be thought of as separate functions," Rapp said. "You might have thought that they would be closer together and harder to tease apart."
Though science knows quite a bit about how the brain handles reading, these findings offer some of the first clear evidence of how it spells, an understanding that could lead to improved behavioral treatments after brain damage and more effective ways to teach spelling.
Rapp's co-authors are Johns Hopkins postdoctoral fellow Jeremy Purcell; School of Medicine professor Argye E. Hillis; Rita Capasso of S.C.A. Associates in Rome, Italy; and Gabriele Miceli, a professor at University of Trento, Italy.
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants DC012283 and DC05375.
Story Source:
Materials provided by Johns Hopkins University. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.
Journal Reference:
- Brenda Rapp, Jeremy Purcell, Argye E. Hillis, Rita Capasso, Gabriele Miceli. Neural bases of orthographic long-term memory and working memory in dysgraphia. Brain, 2016; 139 (2): 588 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awv348
Cite This Page: