New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Training improves recognition of quickly presented objects

Date:
July 9, 2012
Source:
Brown University
Summary:
"Attentional blink" is the term psychologists use to describe our inability to recognize a second important object if we see it less than half a second after a first one. It always seemed impossible to overcome, but psychologists now report they've found a way.
Share:
FULL STORY

"Attentional blink" is the term psychologists use to describe our inability to recognize a second important object if we see it less than half a second after a first one. It always seemed impossible to overcome, but in a new paper in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Brown University psychologists report they've found a way.

So far it has seemed an irreparable limitation of human perception that we strain to perceive things in the very rapid succession of, say, less than half a second. Psychologists call this deficit "attentional blink." We'll notice that first car spinning out in our path, but maybe not register the one immediately beyond it. It turns out, we can learn to do better after all. In a new study researchers now based at Brown University overcame the blink with just a little bit of training that was never been tried before.

"A color change can be very conspicuous. If all items are black and white and all of a sudden a color item is shown, you pay attention to that." Credit: Mike Cohea/Brown University"Attention is a very important component of visual perception," said Takeo Watanabe, professor of cognitive, linguistic and psychological sciences at Brown. "One of the best ways to enhance our visual ability is to improve our attentional function."

Watanabe and his team were at Boston University when they performed experiments described in a paper published the week of July 9 in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. The bottom line of the research is that making the second target object a distinct color is enough to train people to switch their attention more quickly than they could before. After that, they can perceive a second target object presented as quickly as a fifth of a second later, even when it isn't distinctly colored.

Train the brain

In the first and most important experiment, the researchers sat 10 people down at a computer and showed them a rapid-fire sequence of many white-on-black letters and just two white-on-black numbers. The characters would appear and disappear within a tenth of a second. People were then supposed to type the numbers they saw. In one set of sequences the numbers were spaced only two characters, or a fifth of a second, apart. In another set the numbers were spaced apart by six characters, or more than half a second. People performed hundreds of rounds of each task.

Before training, which also lasted hundreds of rounds, people were much more likely to get the second number right when it was presented more than half a second after the first. If it was presented less than half a second later, there was a measurable attentional blink effect.

To train people, Watanabe and his team made only one difference: They colored the second number red.

"A color change can be very conspicuous," Watanabe said. "If all items are black and white and all of a sudden a color item is shown, you pay attention to that."

After training, the researchers went back to presenting the subjects with the same kind of black and white letter-and-numbers sequences for two more days. In the more rapid sequence, the trained subjects were able to get the second number right much more often, almost exactly as during the sequence with a longer time between numbers. Attentional blink was almost completely gone.

In a second experiment they invited the subjects back an average of two-and-a-half months later. They found the perception performance increase was still in place.

To test whether the color training was effective, rather than the mere repetition of the training, the team ran a training session for six new participants in which the second target number was not colored. Then the scientists measured the volunteers' performance. Without the increase in salience provided by color, the subjects didn't show an improved ability to spot the second rapidly presented target number.

The researchers also tested to determine whether the mere presence of color helped extinguish attentional blink, even when it was not linked to the second target number. They asked eight new people to undergo a training in which the second target varied in when it was presented, but that the second character after the first stimulus was colored red, whether it was the target or not. People with this training also failed to improve.

"The results of experiments 3 and 4 together indicate that [attentional blink] is eliminated only when [target 2] is consistently attended to during training," the authors wrote.

That said, the effects of training were shown in successive experiments to take hold even when the experimenters varied the time interval between the first and second target numbers, and when the numbers were masked not by letters, but instead by gibberish characters.

Faster processing or attention switching

Finally, the researchers ran the original experiment with nine volunteers in an fMRI machine to see what was going on in the brain before and after people received training. The goal was to see if training helped them process quicker targets like they do with the more slowly spaced targets or whether they pay attention differently. If the improvement came from processing, the researchers reasoned, they'd expect to see to brain activity when handling the more rapid sequence bear a greater resemblance to the brain activity during the slower sequence.

Instead they saw no such increased correlation in four parts of the brain. This gave credence, the authors concluded, to the alternative explanation, which was that people were deciding to switch their attention more quickly after training, rather than processing the visual stimuli faster.

"Attention is a cognitive component of visual perception," Watanabe said. "We have shown that even a higher cognitive component of visual processing can be improved."

In addition to Watanabe, other authors are Hoon Choi, Li-Hung Chang, and Kazuhisa Shibata of Brown, and Yuka Sasaki of Brown and Massachusetts General Hospital.

Several grants from the National Institutes of Health and the National Center for Research Resources funded the research.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Brown University. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. H. Choi, L.-H. Chang, K. Shibata, Y. Sasaki, T. Watanabe. Resetting capacity limitations revealed by long-lasting elimination of attentional blink through training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2012; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1203972109

Cite This Page:

Brown University. "Training improves recognition of quickly presented objects." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 9 July 2012. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/07/120709155413.htm>.
Brown University. (2012, July 9). Training improves recognition of quickly presented objects. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 25, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/07/120709155413.htm
Brown University. "Training improves recognition of quickly presented objects." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/07/120709155413.htm (accessed December 25, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES