New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

When the color we see isn't the color we remember

Date:
June 2, 2015
Source:
Johns Hopkins University
Summary:
Though people can distinguish among millions of colors, we have trouble remembering specific shades because our brains tend to store what we've seen as one of just a few basic hues.
Share:
FULL STORY

Though people can distinguish among millions of colors, we have trouble remembering specific shades because our brains tend to store what we've seen as one of just a few basic hues, a Johns Hopkins University-led team discovered.

In a new paper published online by the Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, researchers led by cognitive psychologist Jonathan Flombaum dispute standard assumptions about memory, demonstrating for the first time that people's memories for color are biased in favor of "best" versions of basic colors over the colors they actually saw.

For example, there's azure, there's navy, there's cobalt and ultramarine. The human brain is sensitive to the differences between these hues -- we can, after all, tell them apart. But when storing them in memory, people label all of these various colors as "blue," the researchers found. The same thing goes for shades of green, pink, purple, etc. This is why, Flombaum said, someone would have trouble glancing at the color of his living room and then trying to match it at the paint store.

"Trying to pick out a color for touch-ups, I'd end up making a mistake," said Flombaum, an assistant professor of psychological and brain sciences at Johns Hopkins. "This is because I'd misremember my wall as more prototypically blue. It could be a green as far as Sherwin-Williams is concerned, but I remember it as blue."

Flombaum, working with cognitive scientists Gi-Yeul Bae of the University of California, Davis, Maria Olkkonen of the University of Pennsylvania and Sarah R. Allred of Rutgers University, demonstrated that what seems like a difference in the memorability of certain colors is actually the result of the brain's tendency to categorize colors. People remember colors more accurately, they found, when the colors are good examples of their respective categories.

The team established this color bias and its consequences through a series of experiments. First, the researchers asked subjects to look at a color wheel made up of 180 different hues, and to find the "best" examples of blue, pink, green, purple, orange and yellow. Next they conducted a memory experiment with a different group of participants. These participants were shown a colored square for one-tenth of a second. They were asked to try to remember it, looking at a blank screen for a little less than one second, and then asked to find the color on the color wheel featuring the 180 hues.

When attempting to match hues, all subjects tended to err on the side of the basic, "best" colors, but the bias toward the archetypes amplified considerably when subjects had to remember the hue, even for less than a second.

"We can differentiate millions of colors, but to store this information, our brain has a trick," Flombaum said. "We tag the color with a coarse label. That then makes our memories more biased, but still pretty useful."

The findings have broad implications for the understanding of visual working memory. When faced with a multitude of something -- colors, birds, faces -- people tend to remember them later as more prototypical, Flombaum said. It's not that the brain "doesn't have enough space" to remember the millions of options, he said, it's that the mind tries to reconcile those precise details with more limited, language-driven categories. So an object that's teal might be remembered as more "blue" or more "green," while a coral object might be remembered as more "pink" or more "orange."

"We have very precise perception of color in the brain, but when we have to pick that color out in the world," Flombaum said, "there's a voice that says, "It's blue," and that affects what we end up thinking we saw."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Johns Hopkins University. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Gi-Yeul Bae, Maria Olkkonen, Sarah R. Allred, Jonathan I. Flombaum. Why Some Colors Appear More Memorable Than Others: A Model Combining Categories and Particulars in Color Working Memory.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2015; DOI: 10.1037/xge0000076

Cite This Page:

Johns Hopkins University. "When the color we see isn't the color we remember." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 2 June 2015. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/06/150602125718.htm>.
Johns Hopkins University. (2015, June 2). When the color we see isn't the color we remember. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 22, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/06/150602125718.htm
Johns Hopkins University. "When the color we see isn't the color we remember." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/06/150602125718.htm (accessed December 22, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES