New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Some like it loud: Warning coloration paved the way for louder, more complex calls in poisonous frogs

Date:
October 25, 2014
Source:
National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent)
Summary:
Species of poison frogs that utilize bright warning coloration as protection from predators are more likely to develop louder, more complex calls than relatives that rely on camouflage. New research indicates that because these visual cues establish certain species as unsavory prey, they are free to make noisy calls in plain sight and better attract possible mates.
Share:
FULL STORY

Frogs are well-known for being among the loudest amphibians, but new research indicates that the development of this trait followed another: bright coloration. Scientists have found that the telltale colors of some poisonous frog species established them as an unappetizing option for would-be predators before the frogs evolved their elaborate songs. As a result, these initial warning signals allowed different species to diversify their calls over time.

Zoologists at the National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent), the University of British Columbia, and other research universities assembled an acoustic database to analyze more than 16,000 calls from 172 species within the poison frog family, Dendrobatidae. The paper, which will appear in the December issue of the Proceedings of the Royal Society B, is now available online.

The study included both frogs that display bright colors and others that rely on camouflage for protection. Each call was examined in terms of pitch and duration, and researchers also factored in the size of the frogs and their visibility to predators. They found that because warning coloration protected them from predators, they were better able to attract a mate with low-pitch, pulsing vocalizations in plain sight than their quieter, darker-hued relatives.

"This allows the frog to have a unique type of call -- a noisy call," said lead author Juan C. Santos, formerly of NESCent and now at the University of British Columbia. "These noisy kinds of calls, in general, are what the females really like."

Scientists already understood that predators shied away from brightly colored frogs because of visual cues, but Santos and his colleagues hypothesized that some species evolved to include audio signals, as well. Such a warning system is not unprecedented: Tiger moths emit ultrasonic chirps to communicate their unsavory taste to bats. Without a similar ability, frogs navigate a precarious dilemma in which they must either risk detection by predators or forgo possible courtship.

Initially the researchers expected that audio warnings predated coloration, but the results indicate the opposite. Using molecular data and statistical analyses, they were able to infer a phylogenetic tree and pinpoint which trait came first. Their findings indicate that visual traits established the frogs as poisonous and cleared the way for louder, more elaborate calls.

Species relying on camouflage for defense will not invite attention with boisterous calls, while their protected relatives -- including nonpoisonous frogs that mimic the appearance of their toxic counterparts -- can be loud and more nuanced.

"The type of color they have is in the range of the noisy ones," Santos said. "When you're mimicking somebody that's already protected, you have some freedom to be found by potential mates."

These calls require high energy expenditures, but the boon of attracting females without predatory threats makes it a rewarding behavior for males. Less is known about the reasons females are attracted to the noisier males and how they appraise the various calls. Santos explained that if the females are being especially picky, it will drive male diversity by pushing them to create even more complex songs.

"What can the females get from this information? Maybe females -- by being very picky -- increase male diversity," Santos said. A more diverse pool of potential mates increases the likelihood that their offspring will have more advantageous genes over time.


Story Source:

Materials provided by National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent). Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. J. C. Santos, M. Baquero, C. Barrio-Amoros, L. A. Coloma, L. K. Erdtmann, A. P. Lima, D. C. Cannatella. Aposematism increases acoustic diversification and speciation in poison frogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2014; 281 (1796): 20141761 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1761

Cite This Page:

National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent). "Some like it loud: Warning coloration paved the way for louder, more complex calls in poisonous frogs." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 25 October 2014. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/10/141025152713.htm>.
National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent). (2014, October 25). Some like it loud: Warning coloration paved the way for louder, more complex calls in poisonous frogs. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 21, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/10/141025152713.htm
National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (NESCent). "Some like it loud: Warning coloration paved the way for louder, more complex calls in poisonous frogs." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/10/141025152713.htm (accessed December 21, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES