New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

What vigilant squid can teach us about the purpose of pain

Date:
May 8, 2014
Source:
Cell Press
Summary:
Most of us have probably felt that lasting sense of anxiety or even pain after enduring some kind of accident or injury. Now, researchers have the first evidence in any animal that there may be a very good reason for that kind of heightened sensitivity. Squid that behave with extra vigilance after experiencing even a minor injury are more likely to live to see another day, according to a report.
Share:
FULL STORY

Most of us have probably felt that lasting sense of anxiety or even pain after enduring some kind of accident or injury. Now, researchers have the first evidence in any animal that there may be a very good reason for that kind of heightened sensitivity -- or at least there is in the battle of squid versus fish. Squid that behave with extra vigilance after experiencing even a minor injury are more likely to live to see another day, according to a report appearing in the Cell Press journal Current Biology on May 8.

The findings suggest that behaviors that appear counterproductive might sometimes have an advantage when viewed from an evolutionary perspective, the researchers say.

"Many pain researchers and clinicians consider long-lasting sensitization and associated pain to be maladaptive, rarely considering whether it might be evolutionarily adaptive," says Edgar Walters of the University of Texas Medical School at Houston. "Intense pain is certainly maladaptive in many human contexts when modern medical care is available. However, this study provides the first direct evidence for the plausible evolutionary hypothesis that sensitization mechanisms -- which in some animals are known to promote pain -- have been shaped by strong evolutionary selection pressures, including pressures from predators."

Walters and first author of the study Robyn Crook had an interest in the evolution of mechanisms associated with lasting pain. They realized they had a rare opportunity to study related behaviors through the interaction of squid with their natural black sea bass predators.

"Squid perform a stepwise and quite stereotyped sequence of defensive behaviors when they feel threatened, often starting when the predator is still quite distant," Crook explains. "Because we can grade their responses from low to high levels of perceived danger, it gives us a way to measure how injured and normal squid assess danger differently as a predator approaches them and initiates an attack."

In their observations of squid and black sea bass swimming freely in laboratory tanks, the researchers found that squid could get around perfectly well after injury to one of their arms. But they were still at a considerable disadvantage under those circumstances, as the bass continued to pursue injured squid over their uninjured fellows, and from greater distances, too. Once the injured squid became the subject of pursuit, they acted more defensively as well.

What the researchers found most intriguing was the survival value of heightened vigilance to injured squid. They found that squid treated briefly with anesthetic (which prevented their nervous systems from registering their injuries) failed to respond with enhanced defensive behaviors that otherwise would have helped keep them alive.

The sensory activity driving the squids' heightened vigilance may be similar to sensory processes that trigger pain after injury in humans, but the researchers say there is no evidence that squid feel what we humans would consider pain. Nevertheless, the findings in squid suggest a whole new way to think about our own human reactions to injury and pain, the researchers say.

"If we can understand more about what the natural, 'intended' purpose of nociceptive sensitization is, we might be in a better position to find new ways to treat its pathological expression in humans," Crook says.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Cell Press. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Robyn J. Crook, Katharine Dickson, Roger T. Hanlon, Edgar T. Walters. Nociceptive Sensitization Reduces Predation Risk. Current Biology, 2014; DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.043

Cite This Page:

Cell Press. "What vigilant squid can teach us about the purpose of pain." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 8 May 2014. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/05/140508121345.htm>.
Cell Press. (2014, May 8). What vigilant squid can teach us about the purpose of pain. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 23, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/05/140508121345.htm
Cell Press. "What vigilant squid can teach us about the purpose of pain." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/05/140508121345.htm (accessed December 23, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES