New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Researchers find causality in the eye of the beholder

Date:
January 10, 2013
Source:
New York University
Summary:
We rely on our visual system more heavily than previously thought in determining the causality of events. A team of researchers has shown that, in making judgments about causality, we don't always need to use cognitive reasoning. In some cases, our visual brain -- the brain areas that process what the eyes sense -- can make these judgments rapidly and automatically.
Share:
FULL STORY

We rely on our visual system more heavily than previously thought in determining the causality of events. A team of researchers has shown that, in making judgments about causality, we don't always need to use cognitive reasoning. In some cases, our visual brain -- the brain areas that process what the eyes sense -- can make these judgments rapidly and automatically.

The study appears in the latest issue of the journal Current Biology.

"Our study reveals that causality can be computed at an early level in the visual system," said Martin Rolfs, who conducted much of the research as a post-doctoral fellow in NYU's Department of Psychology. "This finding ends a long-standing debate over how some visual events are processed: we show that our eyes can quickly make assessments about cause-and-effect -- without the help of our cognitive systems."

Rolfs is currently a research group leader at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience and the Department of Psychology of Berlin's Humboldt University. The study's other co-authors were Michael Dambacher, post-doctoral researcher at the universities of Potsdam and Konstanz, and Patrick Cavanagh, professor at Université Paris Descartes.

We frequently make rapid judgments of causality ("The ball knocked the glass off the table"), animacy ("Look out, that thing is alive!"), or intention ("He meant to help her"). These judgments are complex enough that many believe that substantial cognitive reasoning is required -- we need our brains to tell us what our eyes have seen. However, some judgments are so rapid and effortless that they "feel" perceptual -- we can make them using only our visual systems, with no thinking required.

It is not yet clear which judgments require significant cognitive processing and which may be mediated solely by our visual system. In the Current Biology study, the researchers investigated one of these -- causality judgments -- in an effort to better understand the division of labor between visual and cognitive processes.

Their experiments centered on isolating how we perceive causality -- i.e., where one event apparently triggers the next. The perception of causality generally involves two components, one that is stimulus based and one that is inference based. First, to see causal structure between two events, these events need to follow each other with little delay and typically require contact -- for instance, a glass immediately falling off a table after being knocked over. This is the stimulus-based component of perceptual causality.

The second component is an inference by which two events are merged into one: rather than seeing one object stopping and a second one starting on its own, there is a continuity of action that is transferred from the first object to the second -- just as in billiards where one ball transfers its motion to another ball.

To test how the brain determines causality, the researchers used an "adaptation" procedure that is often employed to uncover neural mechanisms through visual after-effects -- changes in what observers see. The visual system has been shown to quickly change sensitivity to stimuli that are continuously presented: after staring at a red spot continuously, a white wall will appear to have a greenish spot; after seeing a texture moving up continuously, a stationary wall will appear to move down.

Using this adaptation approach in a series of experiments, the researchers found that repeated exposure to the causal events -- collisions -- test events appeared less causal. Conversely, adapting to non-causal events had little effect. These findings indicate that certain causal judgments show the classic properties of visual processing (i.e., adaptation) and appear to be determined in the visual system without input from cognition. Notably, their experimental results showed that the after-effects moved when the eyes moved, just as the green after-image from adapting to red moves when we move our eyes. Only visual and no cognitive processes would show this specificity to eye-centered reference frame.

The finding, the researchers concluded, provides strong evidence that in some cases, the understanding of action -- causality, animacy, and intention -- is encoded on a perceptual level rather than on a cognitive one.

The study was supported by grants from the EU Seventh Framework Programme (Marie Curie IOF 235625), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (RO 3579/2-1, FOR868/1), the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (FKZ 01GQ1001A), and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche Chaire d'Excellence.


Story Source:

Materials provided by New York University. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Martin Rolfs, Michael Dambacher, and Patrick Cavanagh. Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality. Current Biology, 2013; DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.017

Cite This Page:

New York University. "Researchers find causality in the eye of the beholder." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 10 January 2013. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/01/130110121028.htm>.
New York University. (2013, January 10). Researchers find causality in the eye of the beholder. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 22, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/01/130110121028.htm
New York University. "Researchers find causality in the eye of the beholder." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2013/01/130110121028.htm (accessed December 22, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES